注册 登录
滑铁卢中文论坛 返回首页

风萧萧的个人空间 http://waterloobbs.ca/bbs/?61910 [收藏] [复制] [分享] [RSS]

日志

道德价值观根植人脑 Core values rooted in the brain

已有 456 次阅读2016-1-13 15:27 |个人分类:medicine| activate, stories, values, 价值观, about

Deep thinking in the brain may be spurred by stories with moral quandaries

Brain scans show that stories that force us to think about our deepest values activate a region of the brain once thought to be its autopilot, according to USC Dornsife researchers.

The default-mode network of the brain shows increased activity when subjects read stories that deal with their core values. Image courtesy of Sarah Gimbel/USC.
The default-mode network of the brain shows increased activity when subjects read stories that deal with their core values. Image courtesy of Sarah Gimbel/USC.

Everyone has at least a few non-negotiable values. These are the things that, no matter what the circumstance, you’d never compromise for any reason — such as “I’d never hurt a child,” or “I’m against the death penalty.”

Real-time brain scans show that when people read stories that deal with these core, protected values, the “default mode network” in their brains activates.

This network was once thought of as merely the brain’s autopilot, since it has been shown to be active when you’re not engaged by anything in the outside world. Studies like this one, however, suggest that it’s actually working to find meaning in the narratives.

“The brain is devoting a huge amount of energy to whatever that network is doing. We need to understand why,” said Jonas Kaplan of the USC Dornsife Brain and Creativity Institute. Kaplan was the lead author of the study, which was published on Jan. 7 in the journal Cerebral Cortex.

Kaplan thinks that it’s not just that the brain is presented with a moral quandary, but rather that the quandary is presented in a narrative format.

Reading blog posts and brain scans

To find relevant stories, the researchers sorted through 20 million blog posts using software developed at the USC Institute for Creative Technologies.

“We wanted to know how people tell stories in their daily lives. It was kind of like finding stories in their natural habitat,” said Kaplan, who is also assistant professor (research) of psychology.

Portrait Right

Jonas Kaplan of the Brain and Creativity Institute. Photo by John Livzey.

That 20 million was pared down to 40 stories that each contained an example of a crisis involving a potentially protected value: cheating on a spouse, having an abortion, crossing a picket line, or getting in a fight.

Those stories were translated into Mandarin Chinese and Farsi, and then read by American, Chinese and Iranian participants in their native language while their brains were scanned by fMRI. They also answered general questions about the stories while being scanned.

Stories that participants said involved values that were protected to them activated the default mode network in their brain to a greater degree. In addition, the level of activation varied from culture to culture. On average, Iranians showed the greatest level of activation in the study, while the Chinese participants showed the least.

“Stories appear to be a fundamental way in which the brain organizes information in a practical and memorable manner. It is important to understand the neural mechanisms required to do this, and this study is a step in that direction,” said Antonio Damasio, senior author of the study. Damasio is co-director of the Brain and Creativity Institute, holder of the David Dornsife Chair in Neuroscience and a professor of psychology and neurology.

Biological roots of values

It’s not yet clear whether a value either is or is not protected, or whether the sacredness of a value is on a sliding scale. But in a nation where political beliefs are growing more polarized and entrenched, it’s important to understand what biological processes lie at the root of these values, Kaplan said.

“People will often hold political values as protected values and protected values are at the root of many political conflicts around the world, which is why they’re interesting to us,” he said.

Kaplan’s coauthors include researchers from the USC Dornsife Brain and Creativity Institute and the USC Institute for Creative Technologies.

Belief in core values triggers a ‘default-mode network’ in the brain

Scientists want to know why the brain devotes a huge amount of energy to whatever the network is doing

Neurosciences, Research, Brain scans
The default-mode network of the brain shows increased activity when subjects read stories that deal with their core values.
(Photo/Courtesy of Sarah Gimbel/USC)
Everyone has at least a few non-negotiable values. These are the things that, no matter the circumstance, you’d never compromise for any reason — such as “I’d never hurt a child” or “I’m against the death penalty.”

Real-time brain scans show that when people read stories that deal with these core, protected values, the “default-mode network” in their brains activates.

This network was once thought of as just the brain’s autopilot, since it has been shown to be active when you’re not engaged by anything in the outside world, but studies like this suggest that it’s actually working to find meaning in the narratives.

“The brain is devoting a huge amount of energy to whatever that network is doing. We need to understand why,” said Jonas Kaplan of the Brain and Creativity Institute at the USC Dornsife College of Letters, Arts and Sciences. Kaplan was the lead author of the study, which was published on Jan. 7 in Cerebral Cortex.

The stories matter

Kaplan thinks that it’s not just that the brain is presented with a moral quandary, but rather that the quandary is presented in a narrative.

Stories help us to organize information in a unique way.

Jonas Kaplan

“Stories help us to organize information in a unique way,” he said.

To find relevant stories, the researchers sorted through 20 million blog posts using software developed at the USC Institute for Creative Technologies.

“We wanted to know how people tell stories in their daily lives. It was kind of like finding stories in their natural habitat,” said Kaplan, assistant research professor of psychology.

That 20 million was pared to 40 stories, each containing an example of a crisis involving a potentially protected value: cheating on a spouse, having an abortion, crossing a picket line or getting in a fight.

Those stories were translated into Mandarin Chinese and Farsi and then read by American, Chinese and Iranian participants in their own language while their brains were scanned by functional magnetic resonance imaging. They also answered general questions about the stories while being scanned.

Cherished values

Stories that participants said involved protected values activated the default-mode network in their brain to a greater degree. In addition, the level of activation varied from culture to culture. On average, Iranians showed the greatest level of activation in the study, while the Chinese participants showed the least.

Stories appear to be a fundamental way in which the brain organizes information in a practical and memorable manner.

Antonio Damasio

“Stories appear to be a fundamental way in which the brain organizes information in a practical and memorable manner. It is important to understand the neural mechanisms required to do this, and this study is a step in that direction,” said Antonio Damasio, senior author of the study. Damasio is co-director of the Brain and Creativity Institute, holder of the David Dornsife Chair in Neuroscience and a professor of psychology and neurology.

It’s not yet clear whether a value either is or is not protected, or whether the sacredness of a value is on a sliding scale. But in a nation where political beliefs are growing more polarized and entrenched, it’s important to understand what biological processes lie at the root of these values, Kaplan said.

“People will often hold political values as protected values and protected values are at the root of many political conflicts around the world, which is why they’re interesting to us,” he said.

Kaplan’s co-authors include researchers from the Brain and Creativity Institute and the USC Institute for Creative Technologies.

More stories about: NeurologyResearch

        最新神经学研究:核心道德价值观根植人脑 

                               【阿波罗新闻网 2016-01-14 讯】

一项最新神经科学研究提示,人的精神核心价值观和大脑“预设模式网络”(default-mode network)有关,也就是说一些不言而喻的道德价值根植于大脑深层原因,而不随着人所处的内外环境的改变而改变。

据美国南加州大学(USC)近日报导,每个人都有一些不言而喻的价值观。无论环境如何变化,这些价值都不会改变,如“我从来不伤害儿童”、“我要帮助别人”等等。

大脑的实时扫瞄试验表明,人们在阅读小说故事时,如果遇到和这些价值观有关系的内容,大脑中的“预设模式网络”(default-mode network)就会活动起来。

该校大脑和创新研究所(Brain and Creativity Institute)的心理学家约拿斯‧卡普兰(Jonas Kaplan)说:“大脑为这些默认模式网络提供大量能量,我们不清楚其原因。”预设模式网络像大脑的自动驾驶员,即使人和外部环境没有任何联系时,这些自动驾驶员也处于活跃状态。

预设模式网络的活动幅度与文化背景有关

卡普兰认为,这不仅仅是大脑遇到的道德困境(Moral Quandary)问题,而且也和阅读内容存在道德困境有关。因此,大脑内与核心价值观有关的预设模式网络处于活跃状态。

他说:“那些小说故事在用特殊的方式为我们组织信息。”卡普兰从网络中收集2000万个博客留言贴,分成40种故事,每个故事都描述一种核心价值危机的内容,如欺骗配偶、堕胎等等。

故事内容被翻译成普通话、波斯语,选择的受试者分别来自中国、伊朗及美国。每个受试者需要回答与所阅读故事内容有关的问题,同时接受大脑fMRI(功能性核磁共振)扫瞄检查。

结果发现,如果受试者描述的故事内容与所核心价值观有关,预设模式网络的活动会增强。增强的幅度与受试者所处的文化背景有关,一般地,来自伊朗的受试者的增强幅度最大,中国的最小。

预设模式在神经活动联系中的分布特点(黄色圆斑)。(维基百科)

预设模式相当于大脑“暗能量”

该研究所主任、心理学教授安东尼奥‧达马西奥(Antonio Damasio)说:“从记忆及描述两方面讲,故事内容是一种大脑组织信息的方式,研究其神经机制是十分重要的,这个试验为此迈出了第一步。”但是,研究者仍不清楚核心价值观在大脑中的具体情况。

据《科学人》杂志2010年报导,二十世纪20年代,发明脑电图的汉斯‧柏格(Hans Berger)在测量脑电图时发现,大脑细胞不只在人清醒时才有活性,而是一直都很活跃,无论睡眠还是休息。但是,柏格的这种看法一直未被人接受。因为科学界认为,大脑和机器没什么两样,在睡眠时神经活动处于功能关闭状态,清醒后才把开关打开。

但是,近年来发现大脑中存在一种始终处于活跃状态的特点,将之称为“预设模式”(default mode),它可能对有意识的行为极为重要。而这种预设模式的来源不明,似乎像天文学中的暗能量概念,大脑的预设模式是脑的暗能量。

Creativity and the default network: A functional connectivity analysis of the creative brain at rest 

Open Access funded by Austrian Science Fund (FWF)
Under a Creative Commons license
 

Highlights

Participants completed divergent thinking tasks and resting-state fMRI

Higher creative ability was related to greater rs-fc in the IFG and default network

Seed-to-voxel analysis further revealed greater rs-fc in the IFG and DLPFC

Controlled and spontaneous processes may cooperate more in the creative brain

Abstract

The present research used resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine whether the ability to generate creative ideas corresponds to differences in the intrinsic organization of functional networks in the brain. We examined the functional connectivity between regions commonly implicated in neuroimaging studies of divergent thinking, including the inferior prefrontal cortex and the core hubs of the default network. Participants were prescreened on a battery of divergent thinking tests and assigned to high- and low-creative groups based on task performance. Seed-based functional connectivity analysis revealed greater connectivity between the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and the entire default mode network in the high-creative group. The right IFG also showed greater functional connectivity with bilateral inferior parietal cortex and the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the high-creative group. The results suggest that the ability to generate creative ideas is characterized by increased functional connectivity between the inferior prefrontal cortex and the default network, pointing to a greater cooperation between brain regions associated with cognitive control and low-level imaginative processes.

Keywords

  • Creative cognition
  • Default mode network
  • Divergent thinking;
  • Resting-state functional connectivity
  • Inferior frontal gyrus

1. Introduction

The neuroscience of creativity is a topic of increasing interest but little empirical consensus. Recent literature reviews have been largely inconclusive on the brain basis of creative thought (Arden et al., 2010,Dietrich and Kanso, 2010 and Sawyer, 2011), raising questions about whether creativity can be isolated to discrete regions in the brain (Abraham, 2013 and Jung et al., 2013). Despite the lack of consensus in the literature at large, an emerging literature on divergent thinking, a central component of general creative ability, has yielded a relatively consistent pattern of results (Abraham et al., 2012Benedek et al., 2014Fink et al., 2009Fink and Benedek, 2014 and Gonen-Yaacovi et al., 2013). Such work points to an important role of the inferior prefrontal cortex—regions associated with controlled memory retrieval (Badre & Wagner, 2007) and central executive processes (Aron, 2007)—and the default mode network—regions associated with internally-directed attention and spontaneous cognition (Andrews-Hanna, 2012).

But because these regions also correspond to large-scale networks with seemingly opposing functional roles (Fox et al., 2005), the extent to which they cooperate to support creative thought remains unclear. Does activation of disparate regions during divergent thinking reflect isolated contributions to the same process, or does such activity reflect the underlying presence of a functionally interconnected network? The present research sought to address the relationship between divergent thinking ability and resting-state functional connectivity. To this end, we conducted a series of functional connectivity analyses in individuals of high- and low-creative ability, and examined the extent to which brain regions associated with different modes of attention and cognition exhibit greater functional connectivity in the highly creative brain.

1.1. The cognitive and neural basis of divergent thinking

Divergent thinking has a long tradition in the creativity literature.Guilford (1950) introduced the construct and advanced a mode of assessment that remains widely used in modern research (Kaufman, Plucker, & Baer, 2008). The most common divergent thinking assessment is the alternate uses task, which involves producing novel uses for common objects (e.g., a brick). A central virtue of such tasks is their predictive power: a longitudinal study of divergent thinking ability in school-aged children, for example, found that the top performers eventually lead highly successful careers in the arts and sciences (Plucker, 1999 and Torrance, 1988). Other work has since shown that divergent thinking predicts both the quantity of self-reported creative achievements (Jauk, Benedek, & Neubauer, 2014) and the quality of expert-rated creative performances (Beaty, Smeekens, Silvia, Hodges, & Kane, 2013).

The predictive power of divergent thinking tasks has fueled empirical interest in the cognitive basis of creative thought. An increasing body of research suggests that divergent thinking involves the top-down control of attention and cognition. Much of this evidence comes from latent variable studies showing effects of higher-order cognitive abilities, such as fluid intelligence (Beaty et al., 2014 and Nusbaum and Silvia, 2011), working memory capacity (Lee and Therriault, 2013 and Süß et al., 2002), and verbal fluency (Benedek et al., 2012b and Silvia et al., 2013). Such abilities are hypothesized to support the creative process by providing the executive control needed to guide memory retrieval and inhibit salient but unoriginal ideas (Beaty and Silvia, 2012Beaty and Silvia., 2013Benedek et al., 2014,Gilhooly et al., 2007 and Silvia et al., 2014). Nevertheless, the role of cognitive control in creative thought remains controversial, as other work supports a defocused attention account of creativity (Takeuchi et al., 2011) and a limit to the correlation between creativity and intelligence (Jauk et al., 2013 and Jung et al., 2009).

Behavioral evidence for the role of executive processes in divergent thinking has received support from electroencephalogram (EEG) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) research. Several studies report task-related activation in brain regions associated with cognitive control, such as the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and inferior parietal cortex (Abraham et al., 2012Benedek et al., 2014Chrysikou and Thompson- Schill, 2011Fink et al., 2009 and Fink and Benedek, 2014). Fink et al. (2009), for example, conducted an fMRI study with a battery of divergent thinking tasks that varied in terms of the creativity-related demands required. Tasks with a high-creativity demand required the generation of novel uses for common objects—the classic divergent thinking task—and tasks with a low-creativity demand simply required the generation of typical object characteristics. Compared to tasks with low-creativity demands, performance on tasks with high-creativity demands was associated with increased activation of the left angular gyrus and decreased activation in right inferior parietal cortex. Moreover, regardless of the task demands, idea generation was related to increased activation of the left IFG, anterior cingulate cortex, and the precentral gyrus. Fink and colleagues interpreted their results as evidence for a role of controlled memory retrieval and internal attention in divergent thinking.

The inferior prefrontal cortex is commonly implicated in neuroimaging studies of divergent thinking (Abraham et al., 2012Benedek et al., 2014Chrysikou and Thompson- Schill, 2011Kleibeuker et al., 2013 and Vartanian et al., 2013). A recent meta-analysis, which included 34 fMRI studies and a variety of creativity tasks, found that the left IFG was among the most strongly activated regions during tasks involving idea generation (Gonen-Yaacovi et al., 2013). Benedek et al. (2014) further highlighted the role of the inferior prefrontal cortex by showing that activation of the left IFG increased with the creative quality of divergent thinking responses produced during functional imaging. In a related line of work, an fMRI study of figurative language production, employing divergent thinking tasks that required producing metaphors and synonyms, found that the left IFG was associated with the process of idea generation (Benedek et al., 2014). Taken together, an emerging literature provides support for the role of the inferior prefrontal cortex in creative thought, a region associated with controlled semantic retrieval (Badre & Wagner, 2007) and pre-potent response inhibition (Aron, 2007Dodds et al., 2011 and Rae et al., 2014).

But the notion that divergent thinking is solely a controlled cognitive process is not fully consistent with the literature, as many of the same studies that report activation in brain regions associated with controlled cognitive processes also report activation in regions associated with spontaneous processes. Specifically, regions within the default mode network (DMN) have been reported in several recent studies of divergent thinking (Benedek et al., 2014Ellamil et al., 2012Fink et al., 2009 and Fink et al., 2012Gonen-Yaacovi et al., 2013Jung et al., 2010Takeuchi et al., 2012 and Wei et al., 2014). The DMN includes the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and the adjacent the precuneus, and bilateral inferior parietal lobes (IPL; Fox et al., 2005 and Gusnard and Raichle, 2001). This network has been shown to consistently decrease in activation when an external task is presented and increase in the absence of external task demands (Buckner et al., 2008 and Raichle et al., 2001). Since the initial discovery of the DMN, a large body of research has sought to elucidate its underlying function. Such work implicates a wide range of mental phenomena, including episodic future thinking (Schacter et al., 2012), mental simulation (Hassabis & Maguire, 2007), perspective-taking (Buckner & Carroll, 2007), and mind wandering (Andrews-Hanna, 2012 and Christoff et al., 2009). Creativity researchers have recently begun to speculate about the potential role of the DMN in creative thought. For example, default mode activity has been hypothesized to underlie blind-variation and selective-retention processes (Jung et al., 2013) and internally-directed attention during divergent thinking (Benedek et al., 2011Benedek et al., 2014 and Fink and Benedek, 2014).

Further evidence for the role of the DMN in divergent thinking comes from two recent studies reporting associations between divergent thinking and functional connectivity in default mode regions (Takeuchi et al., 2012 and Wei et al., 2014). Both studies employed resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI), a technique that measures spontaneous temporal correlations between blood oxygen level-dependent (BfMOLD) signals in the brain. Takeuchi et al. (2012)assessed divergent thinking ability outside of the scanner and then measured rs-fMRI. A functional connectivity analysis, with the mPFC specified as a seed region of interest, found that the strength of connectivity between the mPFC and PCC increased with divergent thinking scores. Wei et al. (2014) further demonstrated an association between divergent thinking and default mode regions, reporting increased functional connectivity between the mPFC and the middle temporal gyrus. Moreover, a recent meta-analysis found divergent thinking-related activation in regions of the DMN, including the PCC and bilateral inferior parietal cortex (Gonen-Yaacovi et al., 2013). Taken together, a growing body of evidence suggests that divergent thinking is related to functional activation of the DMN—regions associated with spontaneous cognition—and the inferior prefrontal cortex—regions associated with cognitive control.

1.2. The present research

Although recent research supports the notion that divergent thinking recruits controlled cognitive processes in the inferior prefrontal cortex, the literature also implicates regions of the DMN, indicating that creative thought may also involve uncontrolled processes. The extent to which these seemingly disparate brain regions cooperate to support creative thought thus remains unclear. Because the brain is a highly complex system composed of functionally interconnected neural networks (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009Fox et al., 2005 and van den Heuvel and Hulshoff Pol, 2010), the interaction between individual regions is critical to understanding how cognitive processes like divergent thinking unfold.

The present research sought to address the question of whether discrete brain regions linked with divergent thinking show increased functional connectivity in people of higher creative ability. To this end, we analyzed resting-state functional connectivity in participants of both high- and low-divergent thinking ability. Participants completed a battery of divergent thinking tasks in the lab and subsequently underwent resting-state functional imaging. In light of past research, we were particularly interested in exploring functional connectivity between the inferior prefrontal cortex and the core hubs of the DMN. This approach allowed us to determine whether regions within default mode and cognitive control networks exhibit stronger functional connectivity in highly creative individuals.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

All participants were right-handed, native-German speakers with corrected-to-normal vision, and no history of CNS-affecting drugs or neurological disease. Participants provided written consent and were paid for their time. The study was approved by the local ethics committee at the University of Graz.

2.2. Behavioral assessment and pre-screening procedure

The sample described here was taken from a larger pool of subjects that participated in previous MRI research at the University of Graz (n=91). Participants were prescreened for this study based on their performance on a battery of six computerized divergent thinking tasks. We used a between-groups approach to characterize the intrinsic functional architecture of individuals of high-creative ability. Participants at the top 33% of the sample were assigned to the high-creative group, and participants at the bottom 33% of the sample were assigned to the low-creative group; the middle 33% was not included in the analysis. As described below, the two groups were carefully selected and matched to control for several variables associated with creative ability.

The divergent thinking test battery consisted of three alternate uses tasks and three instances tasks (Kaufman et al., 2008). The alternate uses tasks required participants to generate creative uses for three common objects: a can, a knife, and a hairdryer. The instances tasks required participants to generate creative solutions to the three problems: What can make noise?, What can be elastic?, and What can be used for speedy travel?

After each task, participants were presented with their list of responses and asked to rank them for creative quality. Responses to all six tasks were later scored by three trained raters using the subjective scoring method (Benedek et al., 2013Christensen et al., 1957 and Silvia et al., 2008), an approach grounded in the consensual assessment technique of creativity assessment (Amabile, 1982). The three raters were trained to score responses for creative quality, using a 1 (not at all creative) to 4 (very creative) scale. We applied the Top 2 scoring procedure ( Silvia et al., 2008) by selecting the two most creative responses indicated by participant rankings and averaged the three raters׳ scores. The creativity scores of the entire sample of participants were then rank-ordered.

We also administered a series of personality questionnaires and cognitive tasks. Because divergent thinking is associated with intelligence (Beaty and Silvia, 2012Benedek et al., 2012Jauk et al., 2014 and Nusbaum and Silvia, 2011) and the personality trait openness to experience (Feist, 1998McCrae, 1987 and Silvia et al., 2009), participants completed a battery of intelligence tests from the Intelligence Structure Battery (Arendasy et al., 2004) and the Big-Five Structure Inventory (Arendasy, Sommer, & Feldhammer, 2011; seeJauk et al., 2014 for more information on the tasks and questionnaires). Participants also completed demographic questionnaires.

We carefully matched the groups by iteratively removing participants until they were matched on intelligence, personality, age, and gender (see Table 1). This procedure results in two well-matched groups—high-creative (n=12; mean age=27.33, SD=9.26; 7 women) and low-creative (n=12; mean age=31.40, SD=9.05; 7 women). A series of between-groups t-tests revealed that these groups did not differ significantly in terms of intelligence (p=.203), openness to experience (p=.101), age (p=.287), or gender (p=1.00). The high- and low-creative groups were, however, substantially different in terms of divergent thinking ability (p<.001). The two groups were thus equated on several variables associated with creativity, permitting an analysis of group differences in functional connectivity related to divergent thinking ability.

Table 1.

Demographic and behavioral data for the high- and low-creative groups.

High-creativeLow-creativep
DT-AU: Task 12.32 (.27)1.98 (.30).010
DT-AU: Task 22.30 (.45)1.85 (.31).010
DT-AU: Task 32.28 (.15)1.90 (.30).001
DT-IN: Task 12.10 (.33)1.86 (.24).055
DT-IN: Task 22.37 (.12)1.81(.81).003
DT-IN: Task 32.38 (.23)1.89 (.28).002
DT: Composite Avg.2.27 (.12)1.88 (.10)<.001
IQ115.81 (16.34)106.76 (17.40).203
FFI: Neuroticism.14 (.61).15 (.82).958
FFI: Extraversion−.13 (.94).33 (.90).227
FFI: Openness to experience−.33 (.67).13 (.67).101
FFI: Agreeableness.17 (1.02).01 (.76).667
FFI: Conscientiousness.01 (.82)−.23 (.87).466
Age27.33 (9.26)31.40 (9.05).287
Gender7 women; 5 men7 women; 5 men1.00

Note. The table displays group means and standard deviations (in parentheses). Independent sample t-tests were computed and corresponding p-values are listed in the far right column. Personality variables are Item Response Theory (IRT) scores. DT-AU=Divergent Thinking-Alternate Uses; DT-IN=Divergent Thinking-Instances; FFI=Five Factor Inventory; IQ=Intelligence Quotient.

2.3. Functional MRI data acquisition

Participants were scanned using a 3T Siemens Skyra system (Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany) with a 32-channel head coil. BOLD-sensitive T2-weighted functional images were acquired using a single shot gradient-echo EPI pulse sequence (TR=2500 ms, TE=27 ms, flip angle=90°, 32 axial slices, 4.0×4.0×4.0 mm3, distance factor 25%, FoV=256×256 mm2, interleaved slice ordering) and corrected online for head motion. The first two volumes were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration effects. Head motion was restricted using firm padding that surrounded the head. Data were acquired for five minutes while participants rested with their eyes closed. Following functional imaging, a high resolution T1 scan was acquired for anatomic normalization. Imaging data were then slice-time corrected and realigned using the Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM) 8 package (Wellcome Institute of Cognitive Neurology, London). Functional volumes were coregistered and resliced to a voxel size of 2 mm³, normalized to the MNI template brain (Montreal Neurological Institute), and smoothed with an 8 mm3 isotropic Gaussian kernel.

2.4. Functional connectivity analysis

Functional connectivity analysis was implemented in MATLAB using the CONN toolbox (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/connWhitfield-Gabrieli & Nieto-Castanon, 2012). For each participant, CONN implemented the CompCor method (Behzadi, Restom, Liau, & Liu, 2007) to identify principal components associated with segmented white matter (WM) and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). WM, CSF, and realignment parameters were entered as confounds in a first-level analysis (Behzadi et al., 2007), and the data were band-pass filtered to .008 Hz–.09 Hz. CompCor addresses the confounding effects of subject movement without affecting intrinsic functional connectivity (Chai Nieto-Castanon, Ongur, & Whitfield-Gabrieli, 2012), thus global signal was not regressed.

We then conducted a region of interest (ROI) analysis and a seed-to-voxel analysis. The ROI-to-ROI analysis allowed us to test hypotheses regarding the functional connectivity between the inferior prefrontal cortex and the DMN. We specified six 10 mm spherical clusters with peak- coordinates based on a reliability analysis of resting-state data (cf. Whitfield-Gabrieli & Nieto-Castanon, 2012). Two ROIs were located in bilateral inferior prefrontal cortex—left IFG (−34, 24, −11) and right IFG (36, 24 −11)—corresponding to Brodmann area 47. The other four ROIs were located in the DMN—mPFC (0, 54, −8), PCC (0,–56, 28), left IPL (−42,–68, 38), and right IPL (48,–60, 38). CONN computed temporal correlations between the BOLD signals in the two seed ROIs—bilateral IFG—and four target ROIs in the DMN. This procedure was applied to both the high- and low-creative groups. t-tests and Fisher׳s Z-transformed correlations were computed to analyze differences in functional connectivity between the seed and target ROIs across groups. ROI-to-ROI results are reported when significant at a level of p <.05 false discovery rate (FDR) corrected (Chumbley, Worsley, Flandin, & Friston, 2010).

Next, we conducted a seed-to-voxel analysis. This allowed us to explore whether bilateral IFG was differentially connected to other brain regions outside of the DMN in the highly-creative group. Using the same seed ROIs in bilateral IFG defined above, temporal correlations were computed between these seeds and all other voxels in the brain. t-tests and Fisher׳s Z-transformed correlations were used to compute differences in functional connectivity between the high- and low-creative groups. Seed-to-voxel results are reported when significant at a voxelwise threshold of level of p<.001 uncorrected and a cluster-level threshold of p<.05 FDR corrected. All coordinates reported below refer to peak activations in anatomical MNI space.

3. Results

3.1. ROI-to-ROI analysis

We began with the ROI-to-ROI analysis to examine the functional connectivity between bilateral IFG and default mode regions. This analysis revealed significantly stronger connectivity in the high-creative group in the left IFG and the entire default mode network: left IFG-left IPL (t=3.00, p=.008), left IFG-PCC (t=3.34, p=.005), left IFG-right IPL (t=3.75, p =.004), and left IFG-mPFC (t=2.38, p=.026). This suggests that the left IFG is more strongly connected to the DMN in participants with high divergent thinking ability.

The high-creative group also showed significantly stronger connectivity between right IFG and bilateral IPL: right IFG-left IPL (t=2.76, p=.022) and right IFG-right IPL (t=3.36, p=.011). The highly-creative group did not, however, show greater connectivity between the right

IFG and the mPFC or the PCC at a conservative level of significance (i.e., p<.05 FDR corrected), nor did this pattern emerge at a less conservative significance level (p<.05 uncorrected). Thus, compared to the low-creative group, the high-creative group showed different patterns of connectivity between the right IFG and regions of the DMN.

3.2. Seed-to-voxel analysis

Our ROI-to-ROI analysis found increased functional connectivity between bilateral IFG and default mode regions in highly-creative participants. We then proceeded to a seed-to-voxel analysis. This allowed us to extend the ROI-to-ROI analysis by determining whether bilateral IFG showed greater connectivity with regions outside of the DMN.

We thus performed a whole-brain, between-group seed-to-voxel analysis to examine potential group differences in functional connectivity between the left IFG and all other voxels in the brain (seeFig. 1). In line with previous analysis, the high-creative group showed significantly stronger connectivity between the left IFG and a large cluster of voxels in posterior cingulate cortex (k=791; −6, −34, 34). The high-creative group also showed stronger connectivity between the left IFG and a cluster of voxels in right inferior parietal cortex (k=257; 48, −60, 38). The highly-creative group also showed stronger connectivity between the left IFG and a cluster of voxels in left inferior parietal cortex (k=80; −38, −76, 48). Taken together, the results of the seed-to-voxel largely confirm the results of the ROI-to-ROI results reported above.

Group contrasts of seed-to-voxel connectivity maps with left IFG seed showing ...
Fig. 1. 

Group contrasts of seed-to-voxel connectivity maps with left IFG seed showing increased functional connectivity associated with greater divergent thinking ability.

Our final analysis contrasted the functional connectivity between the high- and low-creative groups in the right IFG and the rest of the brain (see Fig. 2). In line with the ROI-to ROI analysis, the high-creative group showed significantly stronger connectivity between the right IFG and a cluster of voxels in the right inferior parietal lobe (k=154; 46, −68, 50). Novel to the current analysis, the high-creative group showed stronger connectivity between the right IFG and a cluster of voxels in left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (k=86; −36, 4 and 30).

Group contrasts of seed-to-voxel connectivity maps with right IFG seed showing ...
Fig. 2. 

Group contrasts of seed-to-voxel connectivity maps with right IFG seed showing increased functional connectivity associated with greater divergent thinking ability.

4. Discussion

The present study explored divergent thinking-related functional connectivity between inferior prefrontal cortex and the DMN. We used resting-state functional imaging to analyze intrinsic connectivity differences in groups of high- and low-divergent thinking ability. Resultsrevealed increased functional connectivity between seed regions in inferior prefrontal cortex (bilateral IFG) and the DMN (mPFC, PCC, and bilateral IPL) associated with greater divergentthinking ability. This research extends past work by demonstrating that discrete brain regions commonly linked with divergent thinking in the creativity literature are actually more strongly functionally connected in highly creative individuals. The results further suggest that divergent thinking ability involves a greater cooperation between brain regions associated with controlled and spontaneous cognitive processes.

A ROI-to-ROI analysis, contrasting groups of low- and high-divergent thinking ability, found greater functional connectivity between bilateral IFG and the DMN. This analysis revealed greater functional connectivity between the left IFG and all four default mode regions (i.e., mPFC, PCC, and bilateral IPL). Furthermore, the right IFG showed greater connectivity to bilateral IPL in the highly creative group, but not the mPFC and PCC. Taken together, these findings suggest that the ability to come up with creative ideas is associated with functional coupling between bilateral IFG and regions of the DMN.

A seed-to-voxel analysis extended the ROI-to-ROI analysis by exploring the extent to which bilateral IFG showed greater functional connectivity with other brain regions in the highly creative group. These results were largely in line with the prior analysis: the left and right IFG showed greater functional connectivity with clusters of voxels in several regions of the DMN. The left IFG seed was significantly more connected to a large cluster of voxels in cingulate cortex; a second cluster was found in the right inferior parietal cortex, which overlapped with the target ROI in inferior parietal cortex. This analysis further revealed greater connectivity between the left IFG seed and a cluster of voxels in left inferior parietal cortex, overlapping with the left angular gyrus.

The seed-to-voxel analysis also found that the right IFG seed was more strongly connected to right inferior parietal cortex. The right IFG seed was also more strongly connected to a cluster of voxels in left DLPFC—a region associated with controlled attention and working memory capacity (Curtis and D’Esposito, 2003 and MacDonald et al., 2000)—suggesting enhanced cooperation between two brain areas linked with cognitive control in highly creative individuals. Recent research has reported that divergent thinking ability is related to increased functional connectivity among default mode regions (Takeuchi et al., 2012 and Wei et al., 2014). The present findings extend this work by demonstrating that divergent thinking ability is also related to an increased functional coupling of executive control and default mode networks.

4.1. Creativity and cognitive control

The present study raises the intriguing possibility that creative thinking involves both controlled and spontaneous cognitive processes. But how might such seemingly opposing processes cooperate? Jung et al. (2013) proposed that divergent thinking-related activation of default mode and executive control networks correspond to blind variation and selective retention processes, respectively. The blind variation and selective retention (BVSR) theory is an evolutionary model of the creative process proposed by Campbell (1960) and extended bySimonton (1999). Jung and colleagues suggest that blind variation—an uncontrolled process that involves random conceptual combination—may occur in the DMN. Selective retention—a controlled process that involves evaluating blind variation activity—may occur in executive control regions of the brain. The BVSR model thus offers one possible explanation for the present results: increased functional connectivity between the DMN and bilateral IFG may reflect blind variation and selective retention processes working more closely together in the highly creative brain.

Other theories have sought to explain the role of attention in creativity. One compelling theory suggests that a “failure to deactivate” regions of the default network during tasks requiring focused external attention may characterize high creative ability (Takeuchi et al., 2011). Takeuchi and colleagues found that highly creative participants failed to suppress activity in the precuneus while engaging in a working memory task, suggesting that creativity may benefit from the coactivation of executive control and default mode networks. Moreover, a recent study on creative drawing found differential contributions of executive control and default networks during different stages of the drawing process (Ellamil et al., 2012). For example, regions of the default network were more strongly activated during idea generation, and regions of the executive control network were more strongly activated during idea evaluation. In addition, a functional connectivity analysis found increased coupling of executive control and default networks throughout the creative process, consistent with the notion that creativity requires flexible cognitive control (Zabelina & Robinson, 2010).

We propose a similar, but more general account of the present findings based on a controlled attention view of creative thought (Beaty and Silvia, 2012 and Nusbaum and Silvia, 2011). Default network activity is associated with a wide range of imaginative processes, such as mind wandering (Andrews-Hanna, 2012 and Christoff et al., 2009), mental simulation (Hassabis & Maguire, 2007), and episodic future thinking (Schacter et al., 2012). Activation of the DMN can hence be viewed as corresponding to a series of low-level, spontaneous processes with potential relevance to creative thought. Considering the present findings, then, divergent thinking-related functional connectivity between the inferior prefrontal cortex and the DMN may reflect the top-down control of bottom-up processes. In other words, cognitive control mechanisms in the inferior prefrontal cortex may be responsible for directing and monitoring spontaneous activity stemming from default mode activity.

Controlled attention appears to be particularly relevant to divergent thinking because salient, unoriginal ideas can impede the creative thought process. During an alternate uses task, for example, concepts that are strongly semantically associated with the prompt cue (e.g., brick) are often the first responses produced (e.g., “build a brick house”; Beaty and Silvia, 2012 and Christensen et al., 1957). Cognitive control may support divergent thinking by inhibiting unoriginal ideas and shifting attention to different semantic categories (Nusbaum & Silvia, 2011). In the absence of sufficient cognitive control, divergent thinking can be compromised by an inability to exert control over the creative thought process and effectively move beyond pre-potent response tendencies (Benedek and Neubauer, 2013Benedek et al., 2012 and Gilhooly et al., 2007). The inferior prefrontal cortex may thus serve a range of supervisory, executive functions.

Recent research suggests that the frontoparietal control network, a large-scale network associated with cognitive control and decision-making processes (Vincent, Kahn, Snyder, Raichle, & Buckner, 2008), interacts with the DMN during mental simulation (e.g., autobiographical planning; Spreng & Schacter, 2012). Within the context of the present study, increased connectivity between the right IFG and inferior parietal cortex could reflect an underlying ability of highly creative people to exert top-down control over imaginative process arising from the DMN. The left IFG may provide further oversight by guiding search processes and evaluating candidate responses. The left IFG has been implicated in fMRI studies of divergent thinking (Benedek et al., 2014,Benedek et al., 2014 and Fink et al., 2009) and controlled memory retrieval (Costafreda et al., 2006 and Hirshorn and Thompson-Schill, 2006). Taken together, increased functional connectivity between the inferior prefrontal cortex and default mode regions may correspond to a greater ability of creative individuals to govern their imaginations, by executing complex search processes, inhibiting task-irrelevant information, and selecting ideas among a large set of competing alternatives.

4.2. Limitations and future directions

The present research found that divergent thinking ability was associated with increased resting-state functional connectivity between the inferior prefrontal cortex and the DMN. Our approach extends past work by examining the interaction between brain regions tied to the process of divergent thinking. Although the present study benefited from functional connectivity methods, our conclusions regarding the causal relation between brain activity and divergent thinking remain limited. Future work should further examine divergent thinking-related functional connectivity using event-related designs. Such an approach could determine how different neural networks interact during the process of idea generation.

An interesting question for future research to consider is whether highly creative people engage in different thought processes at rest. For example, the creative brain may be more apt to engage in spontaneous imaginative processes (e.g., mind wandering) in the absence of an externally-presented task. If so, this may also explain the resting-state group differences found in the present study; that is, if highly creative people are more likely to engage in imaginative processes, they may also show stronger activity within regions of the default network at rest. One way to test this would be to administer experience-sampling probes in the scanner (cf. Christoff et al., 2009) and ask participants to report on their thought content at random intervals at rest or during a minimally-demanding task that tends to induce mind wandering. Future work could use similar approaches to further shed light on the extent to which controlled and spontaneous processes contribute to creative thought.

References

    • Abraham, 2013
    • A. Abraham
    • The promises and perils of the neuroscience of creativity

    • Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7 (2013), p. 246

    • Arendasy et al., 2004
    • M. Arendasy, L.F. Hornke, M. Sommer, J. Häusler, M. Wagner-Menghin, G. Gittler, M. Wenzl
    • Manual Intelligence-Structure-Battery. INSBAT

    • Schuhfried Gmbh, Mödling (2004)

    • Arendasy et al., 2011
    • M. Arendasy, M. Sommer, M. Feldhammer
    • Manual Big-Five Structure Inventory BFSI

    • Schuhfried Gmbh, Mödling (2011)

    • Benedek et al., 2014
    • M. Benedek, E. Jauk, M. Sommer, M. Arendasy, A.C. Neubauer
    • Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control: the common and differential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity

    • Intelligence, 46 (2014), pp. 72–83

    • Buckner et al., 2008
    • R.L. Buckner, J.R. Andrews-Hanna, D.L. Schacter
    • The brain’s default network

    • Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124 (2008), pp. 1–34

    • Fink and Benedek, 2014
    • A. Fink, M. Benedek
    • EEG alpha power and creative ideation

    • Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 44 (2014), pp. 11–123

    • Fink et al., 2012
    • A. Fink, K. Koschutnig, M. Benedek, G. Reishofer, A. Ischebeck, E.M. Weiss, F. Ebner
    • Stimulating creativity via the exposure to other people׳s ideas

    • Human Brain Mapping, 33 (2012), pp. 2603–2610

    • Fox et al., 2005
    • M.D. Fox, A.Z. Snyder, J.L. Vincent, M. Corbetta, D.C. Van Essen, M.E. Raichle
    • The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks

    • Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 102(2005), pp. 9673–9678

    • Gilhooly et al., 2007
    • K.J. Gilhooly, E. Fioratou, S.H. Anthony, V. Wynn
    • Divergent thinking: Strategies and executive involvement in generating novel uses for familiar objects

    • British Journal of Psychology, 98 (2007), pp. 611–625

    • Gonen-Yaacovi et al., 2013
    • G. Gonen-Yaacovi, L.C. de Souza, R. Levy, M. Urbanski, G. Josse, Volle Emmanulle
    • Rostral and caudal prefrontal contributions to creativity: a meta-analysis of functional imaging data

    • Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7 (2013), p. 465

    • Guilford, 1950
    • J.P. Guilford
    • Creativity

    • American Psychologist, 5 (1950), pp. 444–454

    • Gusnard and Raichle, 2001
    • D.A. Gusnard, M.E. Raichle
    • Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain

    • Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2 (2001), pp. 685–694

    • Hassabis and Maguire, 2007
    • D. Hassabis, E.A. Maguire
    • Deconstructing episodic memory with construction

    • Trends in Cognitive Science, 11 (2007), pp. 299–306

    • Hirshorn and Thompson-Schill, 2006
    • E.A. Hirshorn, S.L. Thompson-Schill
    • Role of the left inferior frontal gyrus in covert word retrieval: neural correlates of switching during verbal fluency

    • Neuropsychologia, 44 (2006), pp. 2547–2557

    • Jauk et al., 2013
    • E. Jauk, M. Benedek, B. Dunst, A.C. Neubauer
    • The relationship between intelligence and creativity: new support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection

    • Intelligence, 41 (2013), pp. 212–221

    • Jauk et al., 2014
    • E. Jauk, M. Benedek, A.C. Neubauer
    • The road to creative achievement: a latent variable model of ability and personality predictors

    • European Journal of Personality, 28 (2014), pp. 95–105

    • Jung et al., 2009
    • R.E. Jung, C. Gasparovic, R.S. Chavez, R.A. Flores, S.M. Smith, A. Caprihan, R.A. Yeo
    • Biochemical support for the “threshold” theory of creativity: a magnetic resonance spectroscopy study

    • The Journal of Neuroscience, 29 (2009), pp. 5319–5325

    • Jung et al., 2013
    • R.E. Jung, B.S. Mead, J. Carrasco, R.A. Flores
    • The structure of creative cognition in the human brain

    • Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7 (2013), p. 330

    • Jung et al., 2010
    • R.E. Jung, J.M. Segall, H.J. Bockholt, R.A. Flores, S.M. Smith, R.S. Chavez, R.J. Haier
    • Neuoroanatomy of creativity

    • Human Brain Mapping, 31 (2010), pp. 398–409

    • Kaufman et al., 2008
    • J.C. Kaufman, J.A. Plucker, J. Baer
    • Essentials of creativity assessment, Wiley, Hoboken, NJ (2008)

    • Kleibeuker et al., 2013
    • S.W. Kleibeuker, P.C. MP. Koolschijn, D.D. Jolles, C.K. W. De Dreu, E.A. Crone
    • The neural coding of creative idea generation across adolescence and early adulthood

    • Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7 (2013), p. 905

    • Lee and Therriault, 2013
    • C.S. Lee, D.J. Therriault
    • The cognitive underpinnings of creative thought: a latent variable analysis exploring the roles of intelligence and working memory in three creative thinking processes

    • Intelligence, 41 (2013), pp. 306–320

    • MacDonald et al., 2000
    • A.W. MacDonald, J.D. Cohen, V.A. Strenger, C.S. Carter
    • Dissociating the role of dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control

    • Science, 9 (2000), pp. 1835–1838

    • McCrae, 1987
    • R. McCrae
    • Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience

    • Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52 (1987), pp. 1258–1265

    • Nusbaum and Silvia, 2011
    • E.C. Nusbaum, P.J. Silvia
    • Are intelligence and creativity really so different? Fluid intelligence, executive processes, and strategy use in divergent thinking

    • Intelligence, 39 (2011), pp. 36–45

    • Plucker, 1999
    • J.A. Plucker
    • Is the proof in the pudding? Reanalyses of Torrance׳s (1958 to present) longitudinal data

    • Creativity Research Journal, 12 (1999), pp. 103–114

    • Rae et al., 2014
    • C.L. Rae, L.E. Hughes, C. Weaber, M.C. Anderson, J.B. Rowe
    • Selection and stopping in voluntary action: a meta-analysis and combined fMRI study

    • NeuroImage, 86 (2014), pp. 381–391

    • Raichle et al., 2001
    • M.E. Raichle, A.M. McLeod, A.Z. Snyder, W.J. Powers, D.A. Gusnard, G.L. Shulman
    • A default mode of brain function

    • Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 98 (2001), pp. 676–682

    • Sawyer, 2011
    • R.K. Sawyer
    • The cognitive neuroscience of creativity: a critical review

    • Creativity Research Journal, 23 (2011), pp. 137–154

    • Schacter et al., 2012
    • D.L. Schacter, D. Addis, R.D. Hassabis, V.C. Martin, R.N. Spreng, K.K. Szpunar
    • The future of memory: Remembering, imagining, and the brain

    • Neuron, 76 (2012), pp. 677–694

    • Silvia et al., 2013
    • P.J. Silvia, R.E. Beaty, E.C. Nusbaum
    • Verbal fluency and creativity: General and specific contributions of broad retrieval ability (Gr) factors to divergent thinking

    • Intelligence, 41 (2013), pp. 328–340

    • Silvia et al., 2014
    • P.J. Silvia, R.E. Beaty, E.C. Nusbaum, K.M. Eddington, T.R. Kwapil.
    • Creative motivation: creative achievement predicts cardiac autonomic markers of effort during divergent thinking

    • Biological Psychology (2014)

    • Silvia et al., 2009
    • P.J. Silvia, E.C. Nusbaum, C. Berg, C. Martin, A. O’Connor
    • Openness to experience, plasticity, and creativity: exploring lower-order, high-order, and interactive effects

    • Journal of Research in Personality, 43 (2009), pp. 1087–1090

    • Silvia et al., 2008
    • P.J. Silvia, B.P. Winterstein, J.T. Willse, C.M. Barona, J.T. Cram, K.I. Hess, et al.
    • Assessing creativity with divergent thinking tasks: exploring the reliability and validity of new subjective scoring methods

    • Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2 (2008), pp. 68–85

    • Simonton, 1999
    • D.K. Simonton
    • Creativity as blind variation and selective retention: Is the creative process Darwinian?

    • Psychological Inquiry, 10 (1999), pp. 309–328

    • Spreng and Schacter, 2012
    • R.N. Spreng, D.L. Schacter
    • Default network modulation and large-scale network interactivity in healthy young and old adults

    • Cerebral Cortex, 22 (2012), pp. 2610–2621

    • Süß et al., 2002
    • H.M. Süß, K. Oberauer, W.W. Wittman, O. Wilhelm, R. Schulze
    • Working-memory capacity explains reasoning ability—and a little bit more

    • Intelligence, 30 (2002), pp. 261–288

    • Takeuchi et al., 2011
    • H. Takeuchi, Y. Taki, H. Hashizume, Y. Sassa, T. Nagase, R. Nouchi, R. Kawashima
    • Failing to deactivate: the association between brain activity during a working memory task and creativity

    • NeuroImage, 55 (2011), pp. 681–687

    • Takeuchi et al., 2012
    • H. Takeuchi, T. Yasuyuki, H. Hashizume, Y. Sass, T. Nagase, R. Nouchi, R. Kawashima
    • The association between resting functional connectivity and creativity

    • Cerebral Cortex, 22 (2012), pp. 2921–2929

    • Torrance, 1988
    • E.P. Torrance
    • The nature of creativity as manifest in its testing

    • R.J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity: Contemporary psychological perspectives, Cambridge University Press, New York (1988), pp. 43–75

    • van den Heuvel and Hulshoff Pol, 2010
    • M.P. van den Heuvel, H.E. Hulshoff Pol
    • Exploring the brain network: a review of resting-state fMRI functional connectivity

    • European Neuropsychopharmacology, 20 (2010), pp. 519–534

    • Vartanian et al., 2013
    • O. Vartanian, M.-E. Jobidon, F. Bouak, A. Nakashima, I. Smith, Q. Lam, B. Cheung
    • Working memory training is associated with lower prefrontal cortex activation in a divergent thinking task

    • Neuroscience, 236 (2013), pp. 186–194

    • Vincent et al., 2008
    • J.L. Vincent, I. Kahn, A.Z. Snyder, M.E. Raichle, R.L. Buckner
    • Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity

    • Journal of Neurophysiology, 100 (2008), pp. 3328–3342

    • Wei et al., 2014
    • D. Wei, J. Yang, W. Li, K. Wang, Q. Zhang, J. Qiu
    • Increased resting functional connectivity of the medial prefrontal cortex in creativity by means of cognitive stimulation

    • Cortex, 51 (2014), pp. 92–102

    • Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012
    • S. Whitfield-Gabrieli, A. Nieto-Castanon
    • Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks

    • Brain Connectivity, 2 (2012), pp. 125–141

    • Zabelina and Robinson, 2010
    • D.L. Zabelina, M.D. Robinson
    • Creativity as flexible cognitive control

    • Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4 (2010), pp. 136–143

Author note: This work was supported by a grant from the Austrian Science Fund (FWF): P23914.

Corresponding author.

路过

雷人

握手

鲜花

鸡蛋

评论 (0 个评论)

facelist

您需要登录后才可以评论 登录 | 注册

法律申明|用户条约|隐私声明|小黑屋|手机版|联系我们|www.kwcg.ca

GMT-5, 2024-7-6 17:58 , Processed in 0.032515 second(s), 17 queries .

Powered by Discuz! X3.4

© 2001-2021 Comsenz Inc.  

返回顶部